Skip to main content

Full text of "The Yurok Language"

See other formats


9. WORD STRUCTURE 
9.1 Morphology 

The morphology of Yurok can best be analyzed by setting up word steins to 
serve as a basis for the description of the various morphological formations. 
In sonae cases the stem will have the same shape as the word itself in som.e 
granonaatical form.; in others it will be an abstraction from the various gram- 
matical forms of the word. The forms of most words can be analyzed by 
reference to a single stem form, but in a few cases separate forms of the 
stem have to be set up. These are indicated in the relevant sections. 

The morphological processes^ employed in Yurok grammar comprise 
Prefixation, Infixation, Inflection,* Vowel Harmony, and Internal Vocalic 
Alternation. 

Prefixation and infixation operate in both nominals and verbals, as well 
as with some words of other classes. Infixation and, to a lesser extent, 
vocalic alternation operate principally with verbals, playing a restricted 
part elsewhere in the grammar. Vowel harmony operates in certain words 
of both nominal and verbal classes. 

The prefixes comprise the Pronominal Prefixes (11,35, 13.141.4). There 
is one infix, -eg- and its variants (13.151), used generally with verbals and 
also with a few nouns and adverbs and with one preverbal particle, so* 
The inflectional elements are numerous and of very frequent employment, 
principally with verbals. 

Vowel harmony applies to certain prefixes, infixes, and inflections, 
according to the vocalic and consonantal structure of the word stem. In the 
prefixes and inflections to which it is applicable, vowel harmony is in gen- 
eral a facultative, not an obligatory, process; where applicable to infixes 



^Cf. 9.21. 

^The use of the term "Process," and of other words that often have connotations of 
temporal occurrence and historical priority, is made for convenience only. Certain 
forms and stems are taken as basic and the structure of other forms is described by 
reference to them, as this makes for the simplest method of statement. No suggestion 
of changes or processes in time should be read into the use of such words. 

^"Inflection" is used throughout to refer to formations postfixed to stems or involving 
the final part of stems. 



GRAMMAR 



13 



o, and a, the two close vowels, 



is obligatory. The vowels affected are a, e, 
i and u, playing no part in vowel harmony. 

Internal vocalic alternation involves three alternating pairs, 
and e/u*. (13.152). 

9.2 Stem Structure 



?/i-. e/i*?!/ 



In addition to the morphological processes just mentioned, Vocalic and Con- 
sonantal Alternation, Reduplication, and Alternation of the consonants r and 
1 operate in the stem structure of certain pairs of comparable words of re- 
lated or similar meanings. 

A number of words may be regarded as having Compound stems. 



9,21 Vocalic and Consonantal Alternation 

The pairs of words concerned may have dissimilar syllabic structures 
and reveal no general patterns except that the vocalic alternation often takes 
the form of the alternation of one or more of the vowels of the stem with a. 



kelomen- to turn (trans.) 
megesik mink 
moyk- to die 
muniponi sharp 
pi*?ih mussel 
pontet ashes 
srekwepi't diaper 
tenpewei- to rain 
wencok^s woman 
we*?yon girl 
weyonah female (animal) 
wohpey- to cross 

wo-mel acorn 



kaljmiaw to turn inside out^ 
megasik weasel 
maykal to wither 
munipani thistle 
pj*?jk dried mussel 
pjncjc dust' 
srelcWahpi-t breechcloth 
tonpewei- to be showery 
wentok^s female (animal) 
wj9yas girl 

wahpayaks- to cross by a bridge 
wohpec- to cross by boat 
wo*?omei shelled acorn 



9.22 Reduplication 

A nuniber of verb stems and a few nouns have reduplicated forms, often 
with an added meaning of repetition, plurality, or the like. Most words of 
this group have both reduplicated and nonreduplicated forms, but there are 
some that are apparently only found in the reduplicated form. Reduplication 
is effected by the repetition, sometimes with alterations, of the first syl- 
lable, and in some words part of the second syllable also, of the nonredupli- 
cated form. 



Nonreduplicated form. 



Reduplicated form 



Verbs 

kelomen- to turn (trans.) 
keiul- there is a lake 
knewe^lon- to be long 



kekelomen- to turn several things 
ketketul- there is a series of lakes 
kokonewe*?lon- to be long (of tufted 
things) 



'e/i*?i is a contextnally determined variant process and may be conveniently catego- 
rized as vocalic alternation, despite the presence of 9. 
*Cf. kalamakal, pistol (revolver). 
'Cf. pan£ah, to be gray (of deer). 



16 THE YUROK LANGUAGE 

10. BASIC SYNTACTICAL STRUCTURES 

10. 1 General 

The basic syntactical structures of Yurok sentences are listed and outlined 
below, in order that the examples in the later sections of the grammar 
may be more easily interpreted. These outlines will be filled in by the 
more detailed statements of syntactical constructions in the subsequent 
sections. 

10.2 Simple Sentences 

10.21 Nominal + Verbal Sentences 

The commonest basic sentence type in Yurok is Nominal -r Verbal. Most 
complex sentences can be treated analytically as expansions of this basic 
type. 

Examples of such simple sentences are: 

nek helomeyelc I am (was) dancing 
pu-k ro*?op the deer is (was) running 
ke*?! ho*>opes build a fire! ^^ 

10.22 Equational Sentences 

Besides the Norrinal + Verbal type, sentences of the Equational type are 
formed of two nomiials or nominal groups. 

wok nelet that is my sister 

wo'?o-t ku tmi-gomin he is the hunter 

kic mewimor neciS my dog is now an old fellow 

nek k^elek^ wis *?upa* well, I am his brother 

10.23 Verbals and Verbal Groups 

Verbals and verbal groups can occur alone, without a subject nominal, 
as complete sentences, 

tmo*lolc I am shooting 

ko*?! nepelc I am eating something 

ho*?opes build a fire I 

tenpewe*?* it is raining 

telogumelc nerpei my tooth aches (lit., I am in pain in my tooth) 

kiti '?e*?gah they are going to have a meal 

10.24 Nominals and Nominal Groups 

Similarly nominals and nominal groups can occur alone as complete sen- 
tences of the equational type. 

k^esi twegoh and it was the coon 
nek ho *?nenah it was mine 
neki*? netektoh it is my log 
mos nek necis it is not my dog 



^^The convention is adopted throughout of marking imperative sentences in English by 
a final exclamation mark. 



GRAMMAR 1 7 

10.25 Independent Subjects 

A type of sentence is found in which a nominal or nominal group stands 
as a kind of independent subject to the sentence as a whole, apart from any 
specific subject of the verbal. 

yo? teno*? wecege*?l she has a lot of seaweed (lit., as for her, her 

seaweed is a lot) 
nek k^elek^ wek new this is my name 
nekah k^elek^ ho pelep we were in a fight (lit., as for us, there was 

fighting) 
nekah kic no'-t wi*? segonki*? as far as we are concerned, it has been 

done for a long time 
ke*?l k^elekw 9olcW kek^ol you have your own fishing rock (lit., as for 

you, there is your fishing rock) 

10.3 Complex Sentences 

Complex sentences may be analyzed as belonging to the basic types listed 
above, with either the nominal element or the verbal element expanded, or 
both. Such expansion is achieved by means of one or more words linked as 
subordinate members of an endocentric construction with the nominal or 
verbal as head, by coordinate nominal or verbal groups with or without a 
coiJrdinator, or by texocentric constructions acting as nominal substitutes. 
Endocentric constriictions may be discontiguous. 

10.31 Nominal Groups 

10.311 Expanded nominal element 

mewah hu*k male »^hild, son 

ni^il pegak two men 

wek \Li *?o'?lel this house <iit., this the house) 

ku pegak mei weik^ew the man from WelkWew 

ku ma-gin ku nepuy the rest of the salmon 

nep no*?oh wjyi he ate two eggs 

nek newok ku pegak wo'^o?! I saw the man*s house 

ke*?l *?olcW skuyeni ite^yoc you have a good boat 

10.312 Coordinate nominal groups 

With a coordinator: 

pegak '^emsi wencok^s helomeye?m men and women were dancing 

Without a co6rdinator: 

pegak wencokWs hu-ksph kem tene^m there were lots of men, women, 
and children 

10.32 Verbal Groups 

10.321 Expanded verbal element 

ku pegak no'?pe'?n mewii the man chased an elk 

cmeya-n ku pegak no*?pe*?n mewil or ku pegak no?pe*?n mewii cmeya-n 
the man chased an elk vesterdav 



18 THE YUROK LANGUAGE 

mecik '>o\i^ ha*?a-g there is a stone in the fire 

sku*?y soninepek I feel good 

nekah ki keih newohpew pek^su numi cpa* we will see each other again 

soon 
ko*?! nepelc *?oikumi ciweyelc I am eating something because I am hungry 
nek ka'^ar wi ki yoh 1 will make it a pet 
ke*?! CO nahcise*?m Icew ku *?nu*k give your name to my child! 

This type of expansion is also applied to the predicative element of equa- 
tional sentences. 

yo*? wi§ tmenomeni *?weikei part of the land is his (lit., as for him, it 
is partly his land) 

10,322 Coordinate verbal groups 

With a coordinator: 

nekah ho helomeye'^m *?emsi ho rurowo*'?moh we were dancing and 

singing^ 
yo*? ho galc^s "^emsi ho cwegin he laughed and talked 

kenimi wo ko'^moyolc "^emsi nimi wo newo-li I neither heard it nor saw it 
nek no*?penelc mewii tu*? tepo-noi ^o ro'?op I was chasing an elk but it 

ran into the forest 

Without a coSrdinator: 

tene*?m *?o*4 wi '?o-le*?m a lot of people live here (lit., there are many 

people they live here) 
kit hego'?omah co*? nu*'?me4 they arrived just as we were making the 

fires (lit., [we] were just making the fires then they arrived) 
ni molcw ho nepelc I had nothing to eat (lit., there was nothing I ate) 
ni moUw skewoksi'^m no one likes it 
kohcew kic '^o co-moyi "^o neskWecolcW after six days he returned (lit., 

six days passed then he returned) 

Under this heading may be included the constructions corresponding to 
"indirect speech" in European languages. In Yurok reported speech, inten- 
tion, and so on, in which the speaker makes a statement about himself, the 
verb referring to this may either be in the second or third person, agree- 
ing with the verb of saying, or in the first person, in this case correspond- 
ing to a direct quotation. 

nek so- kiti ten I think it will rain 

haselc '?owo-k ki hegolc I think I will go tomorrow 

yo*? ha'^m mos ki yegolcw he says he cannot go 

yo*? k^elek^ ma ha*?s ku kepoyurelc he decided to go swimming (lit., he 

decided "I will go swimming") 
to'? hes *?i yese'^m ki yegok do you still intend to go? (lit., do you still 

intend "I shall go"?) 

10.323 Subordinate verbs or verbal groups 

niki to-'^m wenegi-'^nowoi then they all looked for it (lit., then they were 
all together they looked for it) 



? GRAMMAR 19 

nek skewok ki '?nesega*?ageyek I want to be rich 

numi mep new kic *?ukye*?w we were just in time to see it capsize 

10.33 Exocentric Constructions Acting as Nominal Substitute Groups 

ku sega*?ageye*?m kic so*toi the rich people have gone away 
lei wi *?o*le*?nioni wa'?soyowoi those who live here are poor 

10,4 Word Order 

Expansions of the verbal elenaent, other than preverbal particles (14), may 
in general precede or follow their head word (cf. the second example in 
10.321, above). 

Word order nnay be used to distinguish the categories of subject and ob- 
ject, as in English, and in such cases the order is Subject before Object. 

ku pegak no*?pe*?n naewii the man chased an elk 

niewil no*?pe*?n ku pegak an elk chased the man 

nepe*?wisneg nep nepuy an otter ate a salmon 

nek skewoksimek pa*?ah I want som.e water 

yo*? '^ohpu'^m ku ceykeni she fed the child 

ti*?now newo'^m. ku ke*?l kepsec who saw your father? 

ku ke*?l kepsec ti*?now newo'^m whom did your father see? 

cf. ku pegak ku newomin mewah the man who saw the boy 
ku pegak ku mewah newomin the man whom the boy saw 
See 13.141.6I(2)e. 

Where, however, the morphological form of one or more of the words, 
or their sense, makes the structure of the sentence unambiguous, this order 
is not necessarily adhered to. 

nekac (objective form of first person singular pronoun) newohpe'^n ku 

wencok^s the woman saw me 
ke'^l nekah ki ni*go*'?ni (plural verb) we will take you with us 
yo*?ikoh ti*?nisow wis weskewok or ti'?nisow wis weskewok yo^ikoh 

what do they want ? 

In sentences of basic structure Subject + Verbal the order may be varied. 

helome*?y ku pegjk or ku pegak helome'^y the man is dancing 
ho-li? no-loh my basket is twisted 
noTe'^w kepoy your tattooing is pretty 

'I^he order Verb Subject is normal in the following subtypes of this sen- 
tence type. 

1. Where the verb is *?okw. See 13. 141 .1F(6). '?okWs, there is, or 
«^o?okw(s), mokw(s), there is not. 

okWg 9ukepew her grandchild was there 
ni mokw no*?o'?4 I have no house 
ni mo^okw rnec there is no fire here 



20 THE YUROK LANGUAGE 

2. Where the third person attributive form of intransitive verbs (including 
adjectives) is used predicativeiy in place of the indicative. See 13.141.61(1) 

ploksin ne'^yoc my boat is wide 

nimi skuyeni wi *?o*i that man is no good 

Pronominal subjects, on the other hand, generally precede the verbal. 

yo*? neskWecolcW he is coming back 
ke*?! ho teget you were crying 
yo'^ikoh megeloi they are coming too 

But sentences of the following type are also found: 

nimi sloyiketoy ke*?l you have not swept it 

kus no*?ol ki kem nu nu-*?m yo'^lkoh when are they coming back? 

The indefinite pronoun ko*?l, sontiething, someone, precedes the verb 
of which it is the object. 

nek ko*?i nepek I am eating something 



THE YUROK LANGUAGE 

GRAMMAR, TEXTS, LEXICON 



BY 
R. H. ROBINS 



UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PUBLICATIONS IN LINGUISTICS 

Volume 15 



UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS 

BERKELEY AND LOS ANGELES 

1958